Volume 31 Issue 9
Sep.  2018
Turn off MathJax
Article Contents

WU Yu Jia, YANG Wen Han, WANG Qing Xiong, YANG De Sheng, HU Xiao Yun, JING Jin, LI Xiu Hong. Eye-movement Patterns of Chinese Children with Developmental Dyslexia during the Stroop Test[J]. Biomedical and Environmental Sciences, 2018, 31(9): 677-685. doi: 10.3967/bes2018.092
Citation: WU Yu Jia, YANG Wen Han, WANG Qing Xiong, YANG De Sheng, HU Xiao Yun, JING Jin, LI Xiu Hong. Eye-movement Patterns of Chinese Children with Developmental Dyslexia during the Stroop Test[J]. Biomedical and Environmental Sciences, 2018, 31(9): 677-685. doi: 10.3967/bes2018.092

Eye-movement Patterns of Chinese Children with Developmental Dyslexia during the Stroop Test

doi: 10.3967/bes2018.092
Funds:  This project was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China under grant number 81302437 and title 'A study on brain mechanisms of abnormal voluntary control of saccades in Chinese children with reading disability'; the National Natural Science Foundation of China under grant number 81673197 and title 'Brain mechanisms of Cantonese-Mandarin-English phonological processing deficits in Cantonese children with developmental dyslexia'; and the young teacher training program of Sun Yat-sen University (Medicine) under grant number 13ykpy11 and title 'A comparison study on brain mechanisms of phonological processing between Cantonese-speaking children and Mandarin-speaking children with developmental dyslexia'
More Information
  • Author Bio:

    WU Yu Jia, female, born in 1993, Master candidate, majoring in mental health of children and adolescents

  • Corresponding author: JING Jin, E-mail:jingjin@mail.sysu.edu.cn; LI Xiu Hong, E-mail:lixh@mail.sysu.edu.cn
  • Received Date: 2017-12-25
  • Accepted Date: 2018-09-05
  • Objective To compare the eye-movement patterns of Chinese children with developmental dyslexia (DD children) with those of non-dyslexic children as they perform the Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT), and to explore the relationship between their eye-movement patterns and interference effect. Methods An EyeLink Ⅱ was used to record the eye-movement parameters of 32 DD children and 37 non-dyslexic children as they performed the SCWT. The independent samples t-test and repeated measures were used to analyze behavioral and eye-movement parameters. Results Compared to the control group, Chinese DD children presented lower accuracy (F=8.488), slower response time (F=25.306), and larger interference effect (t=2.29); Chinese DD children also exhibited lower frequency of fixations (F=6.069), greater numbers of saccades (F=7.914) and fixations (F=5.272), and shorter mean saccade distance (F=4.03). All behavioral and eye-movement parameters differed significantly among the three tasks in the SCWT. There was significant interaction between groups and tasks in accuracy (F=5.844), and marginally significant interaction in response time (F=3.040). Chinese DD children tended to have lower accuracy and longer response time than the control group in the 'color-word naming' task. Conclusion Compared to non-dyslexic children, Chinese DD children are subject to a stronger interference effect. When performing the SCWT, Chinese DD children exhibit abnormal eye-movement patterns, namely shorter mean saccade distance, lower frequency of fixations, and more fixations and saccades. These abnormal eye movements may be relatively stable oculomotor patterns of DD children performing visual processing, and not influenced by impaired interference effect.
  • 加载中
  • [1] Organization WH. ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behave Disorders. 10th ed. Beijing:People's Medical Publishing House, 1993.
    [2] Li XH, Jing J, Zou XB, et al. Picture perception in Chinese dyslexic children:an eye-movement study. Chin Med J (Engl), 2009; 122, 267-71. http://cn.bing.com/academic/profile?id=ac3e0dff94dfb437e2ed568316cf2437&encoded=0&v=paper_preview&mkt=zh-cn
    [3] Boonstra AM, Kooij JJ, Oosterlaan J, et al. To act or not to act, that's the problem:primarily inhibition difficulties in adult ADHD. Neuropsychology, 2010; 24, 209-21. doi:  10.1037/a0017670
    [4] Lansbergen MM, Kenemans JL, van Engeland H. Stroop interference and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder:a review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology, 2007; 21, 251-62. doi:  10.1037/0894-4105.21.2.251
    [5] MacLeod CM, Dunbar K. Training and Stroop-like interference:evidence for a continuum of automaticity. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn, 1988; 14, 126-35. doi:  10.1037/0278-7393.14.1.126
    [6] Faccioli C, Peru A, Rubini E, et al. Poor readers but compelled to read:Stroop effects in developmental dyslexia. Child neuropsychol, 2008; 14, 277-83. doi:  10.1080/09297040701290040
    [7] Helland T, Asbjornsen A. Executive functions in dyslexia. Child neuropsychol, 2000; 6, 37-48. doi:  10.1076/0929-7049(200003)6:1;1-B;FT037
    [8] Protopapas A, Archonti A, Skaloumbakas C. Reading ability is negatively related to Stroop interference. Cogn Psychol, 2007; 54, 251-82. doi:  10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.07.003
    [9] Kapoula Z, Le TT, Bonnet A, et al. Poor Stroop performances in 15-year-old dyslexic teenagers. Exp Brain Res, 2010; 203, 419-25. doi:  10.1007/s00221-010-2247-x
    [10] Proulx MJ, Elmasry HM. Stroop interference in adults with dyslexia. Neurocase, 2015; 21, 413-7. doi:  10.1080/13554794.2014.914544
    [11] Olk B. Measuring the allocation of attention in the Stroop task:evidence from eye movement patterns. Psychol Res, 2013; 77, 106-15. doi:  10.1007/s00426-011-0405-9
    [12] Smyrnakis I, Andreadakis V, Selimis V, et al. RADAR:A novel fast-screening method for reading difficulties with special focus on dyslexia. PLoS One, 2017; 12, e0182597. doi:  10.1371/journal.pone.0182597
    [13] Nilsson Benfatto M, Oqvist Seimyr G, Ygge J, et al. Screening for Dyslexia Using Eye Tracking during Reading. PLoS One, 2016; 11, e0165508. doi:  10.1371/journal.pone.0165508
    [14] Johnston R, Pitchford NJ, Roach NW, et al. Encoding of rapid time-varying information is impaired in poor readers. J Vis, 2017; 17, 1. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28460376
    [15] Kim S, Wiseheart R. Exploring Text and Icon Graph Interpretation in Students with Dyslexia:An Eye-tracking Study. Dyslexia, 2017; 23, 24-41. doi:  10.1002/dys.v23.1
    [16] Hatzidaki A, Gianneli M, Petrakis E, et al. Reading and visual processing in Greek dyslexic children:an eye-movement study. Dyslexia, 2011; 17, 85-104. doi:  10.1002/dys.v17.1
    [17] Jainta S, Kapoula Z. Dyslexic children are confronted with unstable binocular fixation while reading. PLoS One, 2011; 6, e18694. doi:  10.1371/journal.pone.0018694
    [18] Kim S, Lombardino LJ, Cowles W, et al. Investigating graph comprehension in students with dyslexia:an eye tracking study. Res Dev Disabil, 2014; 35, 1609-22. doi:  10.1016/j.ridd.2014.03.043
    [19] Seassau M, Gerard CL, Bui-Quoc E, et al. Binocular saccade coordination in reading and visual search:a developmental study in typical reader and dyslexic children. Front Integr Neurosci, 2014; 8, 85. https://core.ac.uk/display/43873269
    [20] Pan J, Yan M, Laubrock J, et al. Saccade-target selection of dyslexic children when reading Chinese. Vision Res, 2014; 97, 24-30. doi:  10.1016/j.visres.2014.01.014
    [21] Huang X, Jing J, Zou XB, et al. Eye movements characteristics of Chinese dyslexic children in picture searching. Chin Med J (Engl), 2008; 121, 1617-21. http://www.wanfangdata.com.cn/details/detail.do?_type=perio&id=zhcmj200817004
    [22] Li XH, Jing J, Yang DS, et al. Eye-movement study during visual search in Chinese children with developmental dyslexia. Chin Med J (Engl), 2013; 126, 4306-11.
    [23] Jin J, Ryoko M, Yan H. The Revision and Appraisal of the Pupil Rating Scale Revised-Screening for Learning Disabilities. Chinese Journal of Child Health Care, 1998. (In Chinese) http://cn.bing.com/academic/profile?id=d06f939a1b835e7fc069964fbebc16fe&encoded=0&v=paper_preview&mkt=zh-cn
    [24] Zhang HX, Wang XP. Raven Standard Progressive Matrices Test Manual (China City Revised Edition). Beijing:Beijing Normal University press, 1985. (In Chinese)
    [25] Wang XL, Tao BP. Chinese Character Recogniton Test Battery and Assessment Scale for Primary School Children. Shanghai:Shanghai Education Press, 1993. (In Chinese)
    [26] Vakil E, Mass M, Schiff R. Eye Movement Performance on the Stroop Test in Adults With ADHD. J Atten Disord, 2016. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27060499
    [27] Ling WQ, Bin ZS. Mental Health Test. 1st edition. Beijing:Science Press, 1988. (In Chinese)
    [28] MacLeod CM. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect:an integrative review. Psychol Bull, 1991; 109, 163-203. doi:  10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163
    [29] Scarpina F, Tagini S. The Stroop Color and Word Test. Front Psychol, 2017; 8, 557. doi:  10.3389/fphys.2017.00557
    [30] Song Y, Hakoda Y. An fMRI study of the functional mechanisms of Stroop/reverse-Stroop effects. Behav Brain Res, 2015; 290, 187-96. doi:  10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.047
    [31] Wu CY, Ho MH, Chen SH. A meta-analysis of fMRI studies on Chinese orthographic, phonological, and semantic processing. NeuroImage, 2012; 63, 381-91. doi:  10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.047
    [32] Tan LH, Laird AR, Li K, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of phonological processing of Chinese characters and alphabetic words:a meta-analysis. Hum Brain Mapp, 2005; 25, 83-91. doi:  10.1002/(ISSN)1097-0193
    [33] Siok WT, Perfetti CA, Jin Z, et al. Biological abnormality of impaired reading is constrained by culture. Nature, 2004; 431, 71-6. doi:  10.1038/nature02865
    [34] Siok WT, Niu Z, Jin Z, et al. A structural-functional basis for dyslexia in the cortex of Chinese readers. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 2008; 105, 5561-6. doi:  10.1073/pnas.0801750105
    [35] Prado C, Dubois M, Valdois S. The eye movements of dyslexic children during reading and visual search:impact of the visual attention span. Vision Res, 2007; 47, 2521-30. doi:  10.1016/j.visres.2007.06.001
    [36] Rayner K. Eye movements in reading and information processing:20 years of research. Psychol Bull, 1998; 124, 372-422. doi:  10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372
    [37] Huang X, Han-Rong WU, Jing J, et al. Characteristics of Eye Movements of Chinese Children with Specific Reading Disability in Rapid Naming Task. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 2007. (In Chinese) http://www.wanfangdata.com.cn/details/detail.do?_type=perio&id=zgxlwszz200706002
    [38] Phillips MH, Edelman JA. The dependence of visual scanning performance on saccade, fixation, and perceptual metrics. Vision Res, 2008; 48, 926-36. doi:  10.1016/j.visres.2007.12.020
    [39] Stenneken P, Egetemeir J, Schulte-Korne G, et al. Slow perceptual processing at the core of developmental dyslexia:a parameter-based assessment of visual attention. Neuropsychologia, 2011; 49, 3454-65. doi:  10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.08.021
    [40] Bosse ML, Tainturier MJ, Valdois S. Developmental dyslexia:the visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition, 2007; 104, 198-230. doi:  10.1016/j.cognition.2006.05.009
    [41] Hawelka S, Wimmer H. Impaired visual processing of multi-element arrays is associated with increased number of eye movements in dyslexic reading. Vision Res, 2005; 45, 855-63. doi:  10.1016/j.visres.2004.10.007
    [42] Habib M. The neurological basis of developmental dyslexia:an overview and working hypothesis. Brain, 2000; 123 Pt 12, 2373-99. doi:  10.1093-brain-123.12.2373/
    [43] Peyrin C, Demonet JF, N'Guyen-Morel MA, et al. Superior parietal lobule dysfunction in a homogeneous group of dyslexic children with a visual attention span disorder. Brain Lang, 2011; 118, 128-38. doi:  10.1016/j.bandl.2010.06.005
    [44] Haishi K, Okuzumi H, Kokubun M. Effects of age, intelligence and executive control function on saccadic reaction time in persons with intellectual disabilities. Res Dev Disabil, 2011; 32, 2644-50. doi:  10.1016/j.ridd.2011.06.009
    [45] Brock J, Norbury C, Einav S, et al. Do individuals with autism process words in context? Evidence from language-mediated eye-movements. Cognition, 2008; 108, 896-904. doi:  10.1016/j.cognition.2008.06.007
    [46] Lasker AG, Mazzocco MM, Zee DS. Ocular motor indicators of executive dysfunction in fragile X and Turner syndromes. Brain Cogn, 2007; 63, 203-20. doi:  10.1016/j.bandc.2006.08.002
  • 加载中
通讯作者: 陈斌, bchen63@163.com
  • 1. 

    沈阳化工大学材料科学与工程学院 沈阳 110142

  1. 本站搜索
  2. 百度学术搜索
  3. 万方数据库搜索
  4. CNKI搜索

Figures(3)  / Tables(2)

Article Metrics

Article views(2931) PDF downloads(215) Cited by()

Proportional views
Related

Eye-movement Patterns of Chinese Children with Developmental Dyslexia during the Stroop Test

doi: 10.3967/bes2018.092
Funds:  This project was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China under grant number 81302437 and title 'A study on brain mechanisms of abnormal voluntary control of saccades in Chinese children with reading disability'; the National Natural Science Foundation of China under grant number 81673197 and title 'Brain mechanisms of Cantonese-Mandarin-English phonological processing deficits in Cantonese children with developmental dyslexia'; and the young teacher training program of Sun Yat-sen University (Medicine) under grant number 13ykpy11 and title 'A comparison study on brain mechanisms of phonological processing between Cantonese-speaking children and Mandarin-speaking children with developmental dyslexia'

Abstract:  Objective To compare the eye-movement patterns of Chinese children with developmental dyslexia (DD children) with those of non-dyslexic children as they perform the Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT), and to explore the relationship between their eye-movement patterns and interference effect. Methods An EyeLink Ⅱ was used to record the eye-movement parameters of 32 DD children and 37 non-dyslexic children as they performed the SCWT. The independent samples t-test and repeated measures were used to analyze behavioral and eye-movement parameters. Results Compared to the control group, Chinese DD children presented lower accuracy (F=8.488), slower response time (F=25.306), and larger interference effect (t=2.29); Chinese DD children also exhibited lower frequency of fixations (F=6.069), greater numbers of saccades (F=7.914) and fixations (F=5.272), and shorter mean saccade distance (F=4.03). All behavioral and eye-movement parameters differed significantly among the three tasks in the SCWT. There was significant interaction between groups and tasks in accuracy (F=5.844), and marginally significant interaction in response time (F=3.040). Chinese DD children tended to have lower accuracy and longer response time than the control group in the 'color-word naming' task. Conclusion Compared to non-dyslexic children, Chinese DD children are subject to a stronger interference effect. When performing the SCWT, Chinese DD children exhibit abnormal eye-movement patterns, namely shorter mean saccade distance, lower frequency of fixations, and more fixations and saccades. These abnormal eye movements may be relatively stable oculomotor patterns of DD children performing visual processing, and not influenced by impaired interference effect.

WU Yu Jia, YANG Wen Han, WANG Qing Xiong, YANG De Sheng, HU Xiao Yun, JING Jin, LI Xiu Hong. Eye-movement Patterns of Chinese Children with Developmental Dyslexia during the Stroop Test[J]. Biomedical and Environmental Sciences, 2018, 31(9): 677-685. doi: 10.3967/bes2018.092
Citation: WU Yu Jia, YANG Wen Han, WANG Qing Xiong, YANG De Sheng, HU Xiao Yun, JING Jin, LI Xiu Hong. Eye-movement Patterns of Chinese Children with Developmental Dyslexia during the Stroop Test[J]. Biomedical and Environmental Sciences, 2018, 31(9): 677-685. doi: 10.3967/bes2018.092
  • Developmental dyslexia (DD), the most common neurodevelopmental disorder in school-age children, is defined as a specific and persistent reduction in the capacity to decode written language despite conventional instruction, adequate intelligence, and sociocultural opportunities[1]. Approximately 5%-10% of children suffer from DD in Western countries, and 4%-8% in China[2].

    The Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT) is a neuropsychological test to assess the interference effect. It contains three tasks: reading words, naming colors, and naming incongruently colored words (color-word naming). The first two tasks require subjects to name words and colors, both of which measure the automaticity of readers' extraction of correct speech sounds from their mental lexicon. The incongruent task (e.g., the word 'red' written in blue text) measures the interference effect: participants are instructed to perform a less automated task (i.e., naming text color) while restraining the interference arising from a more automated task (i.e., reading the word)[3-5]. Thus far, literature has identified a tendency for more errors and longer response times in dyslexic children[6-7], younger teenagers[8], older teenagers[9], and adults[10] in comparison to control groups in Stroop-style tests.

    Olk was the first to track eye movements during the SCWT, and showed that eye movements could provide information on subjects' allocation of attention and reflect the impact and resolution of conflict during the SCWT[11]. Eye movements can indeed be used to record information not available through assessment of response time and accuracy alone. Eye-movement patterns show how participants deal with cognitive conflict during the SCWT and how they allocate their attention. They also provide information on the mechanisms driving participants' control of processes during the SCWT.

    Unfortunately, the eye-movement patterns of DD children performing the SCWT remain unknown; few researchers have used eye-movement trackers to study how DD children process the SCWT. However, in Western countries, a huge amount of data have shown that the eye movements of people with dyslexia are abnormal during reading[12-15]. DD children exhibit longer fixation durations, shorter saccades, and thus more fixations in reading than normally developing readers of the same chronological age[16-19]. In China, some studies have shown that Chinese DD children exhibit longer fixation duration, more fixations and saccades, and shorter saccade amplitude than a control group during reading and picture searching[20-21]. Our series of studies has shown that Chinese DD children exhibit shorter saccade amplitude and mean saccade distance during reading and non-reading tasks such as picture perception, visual searching, and rapid-automatized-naming (RAN) tests[2, 21-22, 37]. Therefore, we hypothesize that the eye-movement patterns of shorter saccade amplitude and mean saccadic distance may be relatively stable oculomotor patterns of Chinese DD children performing any type of visual processing tasks, including the SCWT. To verify this hypothesis, the present study tested the eye-movement patterns of DD children as they performed the SCWT, and further revealed the relationship between their abnormal eye-movement patterns and the interference effect.

  • Written informed consent was obtained from the parents of all subjects. The research protocol and content were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Medical Ethics Committee, Sun Yat-sen University, and obtained the committee's approval.

  • A total of 673 students from 2nd to 5th grade (mean age 9.75 ± 1.17 years; age range 6.47-13.53 years) from a Nanhai primary school were screened for DD. Three tests were used to diagnose DD children, including learning disabilities screening, Chinese reading ability, and non-verbal intelligence quotient (IQ). We used the Pupil Rating Scale Revised: Screening for Learning Disabilities (PRS) to screen DD children[23]. Non-verbal IQ was measured by Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices[24]. Reading achievement was measured by the Character Recognition Test Battery and Assessment Scale for Primary School Children (CRTB), a standardized written vocabulary test widely used in mainland China with high reliability (0.98) and validity (0.98)[25]. A total of 69 Chinese children were recruited for the study; 32 were DD children (experimental group: 9 girls, 23 boys; mean age 9.44 ± 1.20 years; age range 7.72-13.53 years), and the other 37 were non-dyslexic children (control group: 14 girls, 23 boys; mean age 8.99 ± 0.93 years; age range 7.55-11.47 years).

    DD children met the following recruitment criteria: (1) overall PRS score < 65; (2) non-verbal IQ ≥ 80; (3) CRTB score less than two standard deviations below the mean for their respective grade (< mean - 2SD).

    Non-dyslexic children met the following recruitment criteria: (1) overall PRS score ≥ 65; (2) non-verbal IQ ≥ 80; (3) CRTB score greater than or equal to two standard deviations below the mean for their respective grade (≥ mean - 2SD). The non-dyslexic children were matched well with the DD children in chronological age, grade, and sex.

    All subjects were reported by their parents to have no history of abnormal learning experience, suspected brain damage, uncorrected sensory impairment, or serious emotional or behavioral problems. All subjects had normal visual acuity or corrected visual acuity (≥ 1.0).

  • Eye movements were recorded by an Eyelink Ⅱ High-Speed Eye Tracker from SR Research Ltd. (Canada). The EyeLink Ⅱ has a high resolution (noise-limited at < 0.01° RMS; spatial resolution < 0.005°; 500 Hz) and few gaze-range errors (< 0.5°). Participants were seated on a modified office chair that prevented any rotational movement, at a distance approximately 70 cm from the computer screen. Stimuli were displayed on a 21-inch cathode ray tube monitor with an average photo illumination of 200.00 lx. A brief 9-point calibration was performed prior to the experiment and repeated, if necessary, between blocks. Each trial was preceded by a brief drift-correction procedure.

    To ensure that the children need not to wear the helmet for too long and to enhance subject compliance, we selected a 5 × 5 target pane, meaning that each task consisted of five rows of five items.

  • The SCWT comprised three different tasks presented on a computer. Task A, 'word reading', was to read words written in black text. For example, yellow, red, blue, yellow, green would be read aloud as 'huang, hong, lan, huang, lv'. Task B, 'color naming, ' was to name the colors of various circles. For example, would be read aloud as 'lv, hong, lan, huang, lan' ('green, red, blue, yellow, blue'). Task C, 'color-word naming', was to identify the text color of Chinese words, which was incongruent with their meaning. For example, yellow, red, blue, yellow, green would be read aloud as 'lan, lv, hong, lan, huang'. We used a series of 25 words or circles displayed in five lines on the computer screen (Figure S1, available in www.besjournal.com). Each Chinese word was 1.8 cm × 1.8 cm in size and presented at a visual angle of 1.47°; circles were 1.5 cm in diameter and presented at a visual angle of 1.2°. The eye tracker recorded all eye parameters and response time. Two professional testers recorded all incorrected responses, missing words, and blocked responses on paper during the trials.

    Figure FigureS1.  Supplement Trial materials. All words were shown in Chinese on screen.

  • We adopted a 2 × 3 factorial design with groups (DD/control groups) as between-subjects factors and tasks (A/B/C) as within-subjects factors. During the three tasks, participants read the lines aloud from left to right as quickly and accurately as possible, with a time limit of 80 s per task. Following are the standard test instructions participants were given (all words were shown in Chinese on screen):

    A: Displayed next are words shown in black. Please read them aloud from left to right as quickly and accurately as possible. For example: yellow, red, blue, yellow, green.

    B: Displayed next are colored circles. Please name the colors from left to right as quickly and accurately as possible. If you see a green circle, you should say 'green', and if you see a yellow circle, you should say 'yellow'. For example: .

    C: Displayed next are words printed in various colors. Please name the colors in which they are printed, from left to right, as quickly and accurately as possible. Remember not to read the words, but name the color in which they are printed. For example: yellow, red, blue, yellow, green.

  • Accuracy[26]: Percentage of correct responses given in each task, recorded on paper by professional testers during the trials.

    Response time[27]: Time (in seconds) that the subject spent from start to finish in each task, automatically recorded by the eye tracker.

    Interference effect (IE)[9, 28]: IE = response time (task C) − response time (task B). This was believed to be most appropriate for evaluating IE.

  • Average fixation duration (AFD): Average duration of all fixations in a trial (in milliseconds).

    Number of fixations (NF): Total number of fixations in a trial.

    Mean saccade distance (MSD): Average distance between contiguous fixations in a trial (in degrees of visual angle).

    Number of saccades (NS): Total number of saccades in a trial.

    Average saccade amplitude (ASA): Average size of all saccades in a trial (in degrees of visual angle).

    Frequency of fixations (FF): Number of fixations per millisecond during a trial.

    Frequency of saccades (FS): Number of saccades per millisecond during a trial.

  • All data are expressed as mean ± standard deviation ( ± s). The results of the eye-movement experiments were extracted from the eye tracker's eye-movement analysis software. SPSS 22.0 was used to analyze data. We compared the demographic characteristics and IE of the two groups using the independent samples t-test. For accuracy, response time, and eye-movement parameters, we used the general linear model repeated measures to test the main effects between groups (DD/control groups) and among tasks (A/B/C), and the interaction effect between groups and tasks. P-values less than 0.05 were considered statistically significant.

  • Table 1 shows that compared to the control group, DD children had lower IQ, PRS, and CRTB scores, and higher scores in the parent questionnaire regarding Chinese reading and writing abilities.

    Characteristics of Subjects DD (N = 32) Con. (N = 37) t/χ2 P
    n (%) Mean (SD) n (%) Mean (SD)
    Age 9.44 (1.20) 9.03 (1.00) 1.73 0.088
    Sex (male) 23 (71.88%) 23 (62.16%) 0.73 0.39
    Grade
       2nd 8 (25.00%) 12 (32.43%)
       3rd 16 (50.00%) 17 (45.95%) 0.73 0.867
       4th 4 (12.50%) 3 (8.11%)
       5th 4 (12.50%) 5 (13.51%)
    Non-verbal IQ 112.13 (17.21) 126.44 (9.96) -4.08 < 0.001**
    Overall PRS score 54.50 (18.05) 99.03 (16.90) -7.61 < 0.001**
    CRTB score 2180.99 (273.27) 2872.35 (298.57) -9.48 < 0.001**
    Parent questionnaire 144.78 (37.53) 81.54 (25.15) 7.55 < 0.001**
    IE (s) 22.99 (14.22) 16.76 (6.36) 2.29 0.027
    Note. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01. DD: developmental dyslexia; Con.: control group; PRS: the Pupil Rating Scale Revised: Screening for Learning Disabilities; CRTB: the Character Recognition Test Battery and Assessment Scale for Primary School Children; IE: interference effect. χ2 tests were used for categorical variables and t-tests for continuous variables. Characteristic parameters were expressed as the size of sample and percentage [n (%)], or the mean and standard deviation ( ± s); Significant results are marked in bold.

    Table 1.  Characteristics of DD Children and Control Children (N = 69)

  • Interference Effect (IE) Compared to the control group (16.76 ± 6.36), Chinese DD children (22.98 ± 14.22) had significantly stronger IE (t = 2.29, P = 0.027).

    Accuracy   There were significant differences in accuracy among the three tasks (F = 21.859, P < 0.001). Significant differences existed between task A and task B (F = 7.606, P = 0.007), task B and task C (F = 27.200, P < 0.001), and task A and task C (F = 44.363, P < 0.001). Accuracy scores were highest in task A, medium in task B, and lowest in task C (task A > task B > task C). Chinese DD children scored lower in accuracy than the control group (F = 8.488, P = 0.005). In addition, there was significant interaction between groups and tasks (F = 5.844, P = 0.005), and in task C, the DD group had significantly lower accuracy than the control group (F = 12.516, P = 0.001); no significant differences were found between groups in tasks A (F = 3.349, P = 0.072) or B (F = 0.201, P = 0.655) (Figure 1).

    Figure 1.  Interaction effect of the accuracy between groups and tasks.

    Response Time   There were significant differences in response time among the three tasks (F = 125.897, P < 0.001); response time was longest in task C, medium in task B, and shortest in task A (task A < task B < task C). Significant differences existed between task A and task B (F = 44.826, P < 0.001), task B and task C (F = 235.227, P < 0.001), and task A and task C (F = 240.599, P < 0.001). DD children had longer response times than the control group (F = 25.306, P < 0.001). In addition, the interaction between groups and tasks was marginally significant (F = 3.040, P = 0.055). The differences between DD and the control group were significant in task A (F = 11.678, P = 0.001), task B (F = 16.863, P < 0.001), and task C (F = 16.731, P < 0.001); the difference was largest in task C (Figure 2).

    Figure 2.  Interaction effect of response time between groups and tasks.

  • AFD   There were significant differences in average fixation duration among the three tasks (F = 61.291, P < 0.001); AFD was longest in task C, medium in task B, and shortest in task A (task A < task B < task C). Significant differences existed between task A and task B (F = 31.320, P < 0.001), task B and task C (F = 49.234, P < 0.001), and task A and task C (F = 124.212, P < 0.001). There was no significant main effect of groups in AFD (F = 3.035, P = 0.086), and no significant interaction between groups and tasks (F = 0.693, P = 0.504).

    ASA   There were significant differences in average saccade amplitude among the three tasks (F = 32.610, P < 0.001); ASA was longest in task A, medium in task B, and shortest in task C (task A > task B > task C). Significant differences existed between task A and task B (F = 11.719, P < 0.001), task B and task C (F = 19.554, P < 0.001), and task A and task C (F = 48.485, P < 0.001). However, there was no significant main effect of groups in ASA (F = 1.608, P = 0.209), and no significant interaction between groups and tasks (F = 0.755, P = 0.474).

    NF   There were significant differences in number of fixations among the three tasks (F = 55.765, P < 0.001); NF was highest in task C, medium in task B, and lowest in task A (task A < task B < task C). Significant differences existed between task A and task B (F = 11.247, P = 0.001), task B and task C (F = 98.450, P < 0.001), and task A and task C (F = 106.866, P < 0.001). Significant main effect of groups was found in NF; DD children had higher NF than the control group (F = 5.272, P = 0.025). No significant interaction was found between groups and tasks (F = 0.268, P = 0.766).

    NS   There were significant differences in number of saccades among the three tasks (F = 58.648, P < 0.001); NS was highest in task C, medium in task B, and lowest in task A (task A < task B < task C). Significant differences existed between task A and task B (F = 13.245, P = 0.001), task B and task C (F = 114.944, P < 0.001), and task A and task C (F = 101.151, P < 0.001). Significant main effect of groups was found in NS; DD children had higher NS than the control group (F = 7.914, P = 0.006). No significant interaction was found between groups and tasks (F = 0.883, P = 0.419).

    MSD   There were significant differences in mean saccade distance among the three tasks (F = 43.091, P < 0.001); MSD was longest in task A, medium in task B, and shortest in task C (task A > task B > task C). Significant differences existed between task A and task B (F = 12.054, P = 0.001), task B and task C (F = 20.327, P < 0.001), and task A and task C (F = 65.137, P < 0.001). Significant main effect of groups was found in MSD; DD children had shorter MSD than the control group (F = 4.03, P = 0.049). No significant interaction was found between groups and tasks (F = 0.223, P = 0.801).

    FF    There were significant differences in frequency of fixations among the three tasks (F = 58.531, P < 0.001); FF was highest in task A, medium in task B, and lowest in task C (task A > task B > task C). Significant differences existed between task A and task B (F = 25.627, P < 0.001), task B and task C (F = 21.226, P < 0.001), and task A and task C (F = 117.364, P < 0.001). Significant main effect of groups was found in FF (F = 6.069, P = 0.016); DD children had lower FF than the control group. No significant interaction was found between groups and tasks (F = 0.579, P = 0.563).

    FS   There were significant differences in frequency of saccades among the three tasks (F = 58.865, P < 0.001); FS was highest in task A, medium in task B, and lowest in task C (task A > task B > task C). Significant differences existed between task A and task B (F = 13.815, P < 0.001), task B and task C (F = 57.217, P < 0.001), and task A and task C (F = 89.331, P < 0.001). There was no significant main effect of groups in FS (F = 2.275, P = 0.136), and no significant interaction between groups and tasks (F = 0.493, P = 0.613).

    Behavioral and eye-movement parameters are expressed as mean and standard deviation ( ± s) in Table 2.

    Behavioral and Eye-movement Parameters (unit) Word Reading (task A) Color Naming (task B) Color-word Naming (task C)
    DD Con. DD Con. DD Con.
    Accuracy*#$ 0.93 ± 0.18 0.98 ± 0.03 0.91 ± 0.10 0.92 ± 0.09 0.74 ± 0.22 0.89 ± 0.11
    Response time (s)*#$ 22.73 ± 10.64 16.13 ± 4.66 28.13 ± 7.63 22.00 ± 4.59 51.11 ± 15.67 38.76 ± 8.93
    AFD (ms)# 289.78 ± 47.35 258.45 ± 41.28 317.31 ± 68.40 300.40 ± 65.61 373.30 ± 85.13 347.36 ± 85.12
    ASA (°)# 4.69 ± 1.73 5.03 ± 0.96 4.16 ± 1.63 4.38 ± 0.95 3.56 ± 1.19 4.03 ± 1.04
    NF*# 60.16 ± 26.41 50.27 ± 14.68 68.34 ± 22.89 59.30 ± 17.00 108.84 ± 45.22 94.27 ± 5.98
    NS*# 72.00 ± 36.51 57.57 ± 21.96 83.63 ± 24.68 69.30 ± 19.79 135.31 ± 55.37 109.65 ± 37.71
    MSD (°)*# 4.81 ± 1.01 5.27 ± 1.20 4.47 ± 1.01 4.89 ± 0.83 4.00 ± 0.81 4.34 ± 0.78
    FF*# 2.76 ± 0.59 3.13 ± 0.47 2.51 ± 0.73 2.73 ± 0.68 2.14 ± 0.59 2.45 ± 0.47
    FS# 3.22 ± 0.71 3.54 ± 0.69 3.02 ± 0.70 3.19 ± 0.86 2.62 ± 0.55 2.85 ± 0.81
    Note. *Main effect of groups was significant; #main effect of tasks was significant; interaction between groups and tasks was significant. DD: developmental dyslexia; Con.: controls; IQ: intelligence quotient; AFD: average fixation duration; ASA: average saccade amplitude; NF: number of fixations; NS: number of saccades; MSD: mean saccade distance; FF: frequency of fixations; FS: frequency of saccades. We used the general linear model repeated measure to test the main effects between groups (DD children/control group) and among tasks (A/B/C), and the interaction effects between groups and tasks. Behavioral and eye-movement parameters are expressed as the mean and standard deviation ( ± s). Significant results are marked by symbols.

    Table 2.  Comparison of Behavioral and Eye-movement Parameters between Chinese DD Children and Non-dyslexic Children Performing the SCWT

  • The SCWT is a typical tool for assessing interference effect, which is defined as the ability to restrain automatic responses that compete with the desired response[3-4]. In the SCWT, the color-word naming task in the incongruent condition (e.g., the word 'red' shown in blue text) measures interference effect[3-5]. The difference in reaction time between the color-word naming and the color naming tasks is regarded as an indicator of ability to inhibit cognitive interference[29]. The present study showed that Chinese DD children had lower accuracy, longer response times, and stronger IE than the control group when performing the SCWT, especially in the color-word naming task. These results indicate that interference in the DD group was greater than in the control group, and ability to inhibit irrelevant responses, as reflected in the SCWT, was indeed more impaired in the DD group. Consistent with our findings, a large number of studies indicate that compared to control groups, DD subjects exhibit more errors and longer response times in Stroop-style tests[6-7]. Literature indicates that reading ability is negatively associated with Stroop interference[8], and that reading and Stroop interference share common processes related to executive functions, such as inhibition and selected attention[8-9]. An fMRI study revealed that Stroop interference elicited activation in the bilateral middle frontal gyrus (BA9)[30]. BA9 is regarded as a center for fluent Chinese reading[31-32]; Chinese DD children have abnormal function or structure (e.g., volumetric gray matter) in this area[33-34].

    Fixations and saccades are two main types of eye movements. Fixation is the duration during which the two eyes remain still. People obtain visual information during fixation. Saccades are rapid movements of the eyes from one fixation to the next. In information processing, people scan the visual field and select fixation locations through saccades, and obtain new information through the fixations[35-36]. The present study showed that compared to the control group, Chinese DD children exhibited shorter mean saccade distance and higher numbers of saccades and fixations when performing the SCWT. We inferred that these abnormal eye-movement patterns reflected relatively stable oculomotor patterns of DD children performing automatic reading, and were not related to impaired interference effect, as no significant interaction effect between groups and tasks was found. The source of abnormal eye movements such as these in DD children is still hotly debated, in particular the potential visual origins. We speculate that Chinese DD children have a visual attention span ('VA span') disorder, which results in shorter saccades and higher numbers of fixations and saccades when performing the SCWT. Our series of studies showed that Chinese DD children reduced saccade distance during reading and non-reading tasks such as picture perception, visual search, and RAN tests[2, 21-22, 37]. We thus believe that shorter saccade distance might be a stable, impaired eye-movement pattern of Chinese DD children in performing all types of visual tasks. Mean saccade distance measures the distance between contiguous fixations, and is regarded as an important parameter of VA span[35-36]. VA span is defined as the number of words a reader at least partially processes during a fixation[35, 38]. Increasing evidence shows that VA span is significantly reduced in DD children compared to controls, and this accounts for a substantial amount of variance in reading speed and accuracy[39-40]. At the oculomotor level, VA span disorder results in shorter saccades and higher numbers of fixations[35, 40-41]. Neuropsychological evidence has shown brain incapacity in DD children performing all types of tasks requiring processing of brief or sequential stimuli[42]. Recently, Peyrin et al.[43] found that dyslexic VA span disorder was related to underactivation of the parietal lobe.

    The present study also found that all behavioral and eye-movement parameters had significant main effects on tasks. For behavioral parameters, task A had the highest accuracy and shortest response time, and task C had the lowest accuracy and longest response time. For eye-movement parameters, task A had the shortest AFD and MSD, longest ASA, lowest NF and NS, and highest FF and FS; task C had the longest AFD and MSD, shortest ASA, highest NF and NS, and lowest FF and FS. Task B received medium scores for all behavioral and eye-movement parameters. The data showed that task C was the most difficult to process, task B was moderate, and task A was easiest, supporting the hypothesis of the Stroop effect. The word reading task (task A) had a higher degree of automaticity than the color naming task (task B). In task C, participants were required to repress and prevent the automatic process of reading the word from interfering with the controlled process of naming the color, slowing down processing speed and causing more errors[3-4].

    There are some limitations in the present study. First, our sample size was small, which influenced the stability of the results. Second, there were significant differences in non-verbal IQ between two groups, which might influence eye movements. To our knowledge, little work has been conducted to elucidate the relationship between eye movements and IQ. Haishi et al. found a negative correlation between saccadic reaction time and IQ[44]. However, Brock et al. found that non-verbal IQ did not influence gazing patterns[45], and Lasker et al. also have stated that there is no evidence of correlation between IQ and eye-movement latencies[46]. Therefore, it remains unclear whether non-verbal IQ influences the eye movements of children. Finally, this research was a case-control study, which cannot explore the source of the abnormal eye movements in DD children, nor the causal relationship between abnormal eye movements and dyslexia.

  • Chinese DD children are subject to a stronger interference effect. When performing the SCWT, Chinese DD children exhibit abnormal eye-movement patterns, namely shorter mean saccade distance, lower frequency of fixations, and more fixations and saccades. These abnormal eye movements may be relatively stable oculomotor patterns of DD children when performing visual processing, and not influenced by impaired interference effect. We concluded that Chinese DD children have VA span disorder, which results in shorter saccades and higher numbers of fixations and saccades when performing the SCWT. Further study should be conducted to explore the causal relationship between abnormal eye movements and VA span disorder in DD children.

  • WU Yu Jia contributed to data analysis, results interpretation, and manuscript drafting. YANG Wen Han, WANG Qing Xiong, YANG De Sheng, and HU Xiao Yun contributed to results interpretation and manuscript revision. JING Jin contributed to study design, data analysis planning, results interpretation, and manuscript revision. LI Xiu Hong was the principal investigator of this study and contributed to hypothesis generation, study design, data analysis planning, results interpretation, and major revision of the manuscript. All authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

  • We thank QIU Yu Xiang, HUANG Ying Fang, and TAN Ren Xiang of the Government of Jiujiang Town, Nanhai, Foshan, for their assistance during data acquisition. We thank the pupils and staff of the Central Elementary School of Jiujiang Town for their participation in the test.

  • There are no conflicts of interest.

Reference (46)

Catalog

    /

    DownLoad:  Full-Size Img  PowerPoint
    Return
    Return